CARLISLE, Pennsylvania: As COVID-19 fans across the globe, some people have been more complacent about the risk of contracting the virus than others.
On Mar 21, New York Governor Andrew Cuomo was apoplectic after photographs emerged of New Yorkers congregating in parks, apparently ignoring mandates for social distancing.
“It’s insensitive, it’s arrogant, it’s self-destructive, it’s disrespectful and it has to stop now,” Cuomo said. “This is not a joke, and I am not kidding.”
Where does this complacency come from? Why are so many refusing to change their habits and are going about their day as if everything were normal?
READ: Commentary: Has Myanmar been in denial over COVID-19 outbreak all this while?
READ: Commentary: How safety measures for the coronavirus could lead to worrisome changes in behaviour
OPTIMISTIC BIAS
It could have to do with a well-documented phenomenon called the “optimistic bias”, which I’ve researched extensively as a social psychological scientist. Essentially, it refers to when people see themselves as less susceptible to risk than others.
We think, for example, that we’re less at risk than other people of having a stroke, getting injured in a car accident or getting divorced. Smokers know that smoking is dangerous but still think they’re less at risk than other smokers of getting lung cancer.
We’re seeing this same phenomenon play out with the coronavirus.
A recent study of more than 4,000 Europeans in France, Italy, the UK and Switzerland showed that about half of the participants thought they were less likely than other people of getting infected. And only 5 per cent of respondents thought they’d be more likely to become infected.
LISTEN: What’s behind the different approaches countries are taking towards COVID-19?
A message about protecting yourself from COVID-19 is seen on an electronic billboard in a nearly empty Times Square in Manhattan in New York City, New York on Mar 20, 2020. (Photo: REUTERS/Mike Segar)
Is that because people are delusional? Hardly. Earlier this month, a study of nearly 1,600 Americans found that people thought their own personal risk was increasing, but they saw everyone else’s risk also increasing. Therefore, people still thought they were personally less at risk than other people.
HARD TO GRASP RISK OF INFECTION
Personal experiences play a big role. For now, COVID-19 can seem distant and far away.
Without viscerally experiencing it – either through contracting it yourself or seeing a close friend or family member become infected – risk of infection can be hard to grasp.
In fact, my research found that people who had experienced an earthquake up close – as residents of Los Angeles did in 1994 – weren’t optimistically biased that they would avoid injury from a future one. However they were optimistically biased about emerging unscathed from natural disasters they hadn’t experienced, such as a flood.
For this reason, I’d expect the optimistic bias for COVID-19 to be smaller or absent in areas with high casualties where the virus has hit “close to home”.
READ: Commentary: No ordinary disruption – a rising generation meets the coronavirus
READ: Commentary: Why Singapore is preparing to tap the brakes to slow COVID-19 spread
In general, optimism is a good coping mechanism. It gives us a sense of control, and reduces our anxiety, when we think that bad stuff happens to other people, but not to us.
But in the case of a pandemic like the coronavirus, if you don’t think something bad is going to happen to you, you might not bother changing your behaviours.
That’s exactly what a US study on the coronavirus found: Personal risk perception was the best predictor of whether people washed their hands or engaged in social distancing. Similarly, research on the H1N1 virus showed that personally believing you were at risk predicted engaging in more avoidance behaviours.
MORAL CHOICES
Risk perceptions can be hard to change, but one strategy is to consider socially distancing and staying at home as moral choices. Causing harm to others is typically viewed as immoral especially if the victims are seen as vulnerable and in need of protection, like the elderly.
In my own research on smoking, the more smokers agreed it was immoral to smoke because it could harm others, the greater they saw their own personal risk. These participants were the most interested in quitting.
Citizens wearing protective masks in the streets of Venice. REUTERS/Manuel Silvestri
READ: ‘Only at the beginning of a very long fight’: Lawrence Wong says Singapore will continue to review COVID-19 measures
What’s difficult with all risks – including the coronavirus – is that you don’t necessarily know or see the potential harm to other people from your own behaviour.
If you don’t think you’re infected, you might not think it matters to avoid interacting with your grandmother. So the uncertainty of infection leaves you open to risk minimisation and rationalisation.
But turning the action from one of personal risk to one of moral choice might interrupt the risk minimisation process and boost preventative behaviours.
It’s possible to adopt an optimistic “we’ll get through this” attitude while still taking all the recommended precautions.
BOOKMARK THIS: Our comprehensive coverage of the novel coronavirus and its developments
Download our app or subscribe to our Telegram channel for the latest updates on the coronavirus outbreak: https://cna.asia/telegram
Marie Helweg-Larsen is a professor of psychology at Dickinson College. This commentary first appeared on The Conversation.
More Stories
Becoming a Foreclosure Consultant
Grow Your Business With the Best ICT Consulting
Choose The Best CRM For Your Business With Salesforce Consultants